skip to primary navigation skip to content
*** PLEASE READ ************************************** THIS PAGE HAS BEEN IMPORTED FROM THE OLD SITE. FORMATTING IS MAINTAINED BY AN EXTERNAL STYLESHEET. WHEN YOU EDIT THIS PAGE, YOU MAY WANT TO REMOVE THE REFERENCE TO THIS STYLESHEET AND UPDATE THE FORMATTING. ******************************************************

Selection among noisy data, a widely accepted practice in neuroscience, may inflate or invalidate certain results

space

cartoon diagrams

Cartoon diagrams for understanding circular analysis. (a) The top row serves to remind us that our results reflect our data indirectly: through the lens of an often complicated analysis, whose assumptions are not always fully explicit. The bottom row illustrates how the assumptions (and hypotheses) can interact with the data to shape the results. Ideally (bottom left), the results reflect some aspect of the data (blue) without distortion (although the assumptions will determine what aspect of the data is reflected in the results). But sometimes (bottom center) a close inspection of the analysis reveals that the data get lost in the process and the assumptions (red) predetermine the results. In that case the analysis is completely circular (red dotted line). More frequently in practice (bottom right), the assumptions tinge the results (magenta). The results are then distorted by circularity, but still reflect the data to some degree (magenta dotted lines). (b) Three diagrams illustrate the three most common causes of circularity: selection (left), weighting (center), and sorting (right). Selection, weighting, and sorting criteria reflect assumptions and hypotheses (red). Each of the three can tinge the results, distorting the estimates presented and invalidating statistical tests, if the results statistics are not independent of the criteria for selection, weighting, or sorting.

selection bias in ROI analysis

ROI definition can bias activation analysis. A simulated fMRI block-design experiment demonstrates that nonindependent ROI definition can distort effects and produce spuriously significant results, even when the ROI is defined by rigorous mapping procedures (accounting for multiple tests) and highlights a truly activated region. Error bars indicate +/- 1 standard error of the mean. (a) The layout of this panel matches the intuitive diagrams of Fig. 1a: The data in Fig. 1a correspond to the true effects (left); the assumptions to the contrast hypothesis (top), and the results to ROI-average activation analyses (right). A 100-voxel region (blue contour in central slice map) was simulated to be active during conditions A and B, but not during conditions C and D (left). The t map for contrast A-D is shown for the central slice through the region (center). When thresholded at p<0.05 (corrected for multiple tests by a cluster threshold criterion), a cluster appears (magenta contour), which highlights the true activated region (blue contour). The ROI is somewhat affected by the noise in the data (difference between blue and magenta contours). The noise pushes some truly activated voxels below the threshold and lifts some nonactivated voxels above the threshold (white arrows). This can be interpreted as overfitting. The bar graph for the overfitted ROI (bottom right, same data as used for mapping) reflects the activation of the region during conditions A and B as well as the absence of activation during conditions C and D. However, in comparison to the true effects (left) it is substantially distorted by the selection contrast A-D (top). In particular, the contrast A-B (simulated to be zero) exhibits spurious significance (p<0.01). When we use independent data to define the ROI (green contour), no such distortion is observed (top right). (b) The simulation illustrates how data selection blends truth (left) and hypothesis (right) by distorting results (top) so as to better conform to the selection criterion.

Teaching slides

Here are some slides for teaching.

References

Circular analysis in systems neuroscience – the dangers of double dipping pdf
Kriegeskorte N, Simmons WK, Bellgowan PSF, Baker CI. (2009) Nature Neuroscience 12(5): 535-40.

cover

Cover illustration

genesis();