In this programme we study basic cognitive and brain mechanisms of selective attention, with emphasis on attentional control functions of the frontal and parietal lobes. Our central question is how the brain builds organized thought and behaviour as a structure of attentional episodes, extending from attention and awareness in visual processing to performance and control of complex tasks. Aiming to integrate cognitive models with neural functions, we use studies of normal human behaviour, neuropsychological studies of change after brain damage, brain imaging and electrophysiology. Basic science is linked to application in a range of areas, including management of attentional deficits after brain damage, individual differences in ability, cognitive training and change over the lifespan.
Projects
As we should expect, fMRI shows many local regions of the brain involved in specific cognitive activities, such as recognizing a face or understanding language. Less expected is the complementary discovery of a constant pattern of frontal and parietal lobe activity associated with many different kinds of cognitive demand. This same “multiple-demand” or MD pattern is seen in tasks tapping stimulus discrimination, working and episodic memory, response inhibition, language, problem-solving, arithmetic and much more (see here). The MD network, we propose, constructs attentional control structures for complex behaviour of all kinds.
With fMRI, the MD network can be precisely defined in the brains of individual people. Often, MD regions are immediately adjacent to regions with quite different functional properties, e.g. specific involvement in language. With identification of specific MD regions in individual brains, we aim for detailed investigation of their functional properties and contributions to attentional control.
References:
Once MD regions are defined, their physiological properties can be examined using a range of complementary methods. With fMRI, for example, detailed representations of task events can be studied using multivoxel pattern analysis or MVPA (Kriegeskorte). Our results show broad coding of many different kinds of task information, including relevant stimuli, responses and rules. In our group at the University of Oxford, neurophysiological recordings in behaving monkeys allow MD activity to be examined at the level of activity in single cells and cell populations. In lateral frontal cortex, for example, neurons show high speed adaptation to code the specific information and events of current behaviour; explaining why, in fMRI, activity in the same regions is seen for many different kinds of task. Collaborations with clinical groups in several institutions worldwide are allowing us to conduct similar studies in human patients with implanted brain electrodes. With these methods we track how MD regions construct the contents of an attentional episode, and track neural dynamics as the pattern of activity for one step of a task completes and makes way for the next.
The components of the MD system are anatomically distinct, yet in fMRI, their activity is largely similar. Using a range of methods, we are asking how different MD regions interact in behavioural control. With fMRI, for example, have tested the common proposal of a processing hierarchy in prefrontal cortex, with most anterior regions involved in the highest levels of control.
References:
Woolgar, 2011
Sigala, 2008
Stokes et al., 2013
Well-known tests of “fluid intelligence” – usually involving novel problem-solving – are of interest for their broad ability to predict success in many kinds of laboratory and real-world tasks. In fMRI, fluid intelligence tests show strong activity in the MD system, and in neurological cases, we find that impaired fluid intelligence is predicted by MD lesions. These tests, we propose, measure a fundamental aspect of MD activity, solving complex problems through division into simpler, more tractable episodes.
To develop this hypothesis, we are examining a bizarre form of error termed goal neglect. In goal neglect, a person knows and can describe task rules, yet these rules have no apparent influence on behaviour. Extreme cases are well known from the literature on frontal lobe damage, but when tasks are novel and complex, neglect is seen also in the normal population. Importantly, it is linked closely to fluid intelligence, and in behavioural experiments we are linking neglect to attentional control.
Fluid intelligence declines strongly in old age. Using MRI, we are asking how cognitive decline links to changed structure and function within and outside the MD system (camCAM, Henson).
References:
Duncan et al., 2008
Duncan et al., 2017
Woolgar et al., 2018
Many clinical tests are used to measure executive control deficits in frontal lobe patients. Because fluid intelligence is correlated with all kinds of test, and because it is impaired after MD lesions, we are asking how far different clinical deficits are explained by fluid intelligence loss.
The results show an intriguing picture. For some common tests, such as Wisconsin Card Sorting and Trail Making, deficits in frontal lobe patients are entirely explained by fluid intelligence. Since much of the frontal lobe lies outside the MD system, however, we should not expect this for all tests. Confirming this prediction, some aspects of executive control (e.g. social cognition, complex multi-tasking) show deficits separate from fluid intelligence. In some cases, the evidence suggests link to damage outside the MD system, especially the most anterior parts of the frontal lobe.
In a wide range of neurological and psychiatric conditions, impairments in attentional control cripple return to everyday life. Their assessment, however, is notoriously uncertain. Our methods suggest a new approach to defining distinct aspects of clinical deficit, and with Tom Manly, we are investigating their utility in patient assessment and management.
References:
Roca at el 2010
The brain’s “default mode network” or DMN is often considered complementary to the MD network, with reduced activity in many tasks compared to rest. A common interpretation is that DMN activity reflects self- or internally-directed cognition. Our research, however, links DMN activity to any major change in the focus of cognition. We are investigating how MD and DMN systems combine in cognitive control.
References:
Crittenden et al., 2015
In several new lines of work, we are examining the potential for training to overcome attentional limitations. A variety of attentional impairments follow damage to the brain, including both lateralized and global deficits. Using sophisticated cognitive assessment, we are searching for clinical improvements through training attentional capacity and control (Manly, Astle).
More broadly, low fluid intelligence impacts achievements in many aspects of daily life. To address such difficulties, we are exploring methods for improved division of tasks into simple, manageable parts, and the potential role of parent-child interaction in promoting this ability.
References:
Duncan and Manly, 2012
John Duncan: Programme leader
Moataz Assem: Research student
Andrew Bell: Senior investigator scientist
Yaara Erez: Postdoctoral fellow
Sharon.Erzinclioglu: Research assistant
Miki Kadohisa: Investigator scientist
Makoto Kusunoki: Senior investigator scientist
Sneha Shashidhara: Research student
Verity Smith: Research student
Tanya Wen: Research student
Danny Mitchell: Senior investigator scientist